Search Mediators Near You:

Memories Serve as Tools for Learning and Decision-Making

  • Neuroscience and Conflict Resolution Blog by Stephanie West Allen

    Below are a news release from University of Texas-Austin, plus the study Summary and Conclusions. For reminder of why learning is important for conflict resolution, see these posts.

    Memories Serve as Tools for Learning and Decision-Making, New Study Shows

    July 11, 2012

    AUSTIN, Texas — When humans learn, their brains relate new information with past experiences to derive new knowledge, according to psychology research from The University of Texas at Austin.

    The study, led by Alison Preston, assistant professor of psychology and neurobiology, shows this memory-binding process allows people to better understand new concepts and make future decisions. The findings could lead to better teaching methods, as well as treatment of degenerative neurological disorders, such as dementia, Preston says.

    “Memories are not just for reflecting on the past; they help us make the best decisions for the future,” says Preston, a research affiliate in the Center for Learning and Memory, which is part of the university’s College of Natural Sciences. “Here, we provide a direct link between these derived memories and the ability to make novel inferences.”

    The paper was published online in July in the journal Neuron. The authors include University of Texas at Austin researchers Dagmar Zeithamova and April Dominick.

    In the study, 34 subjects were shown a series of paired images composed of different elements (for example, an object and an outdoor scene). Each of the paired images would then reappear in more presentations. A backpack, paired with a horse in the first presentation, would appear alongside a field in a later presentation. The overlap between the backpack and outdoor scenery (horse and field) would cause the viewer to associate the backpack with the horse and field. The researchers used this

    strategy to see how respondents would delve back to a recent memory while processing new information.

    Using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) equipment, the researchers were able to look at the subjects’ brain activity as they looked at image presentations. Using this technique, Preston and her team were able to see how the respondents thought about past images while looking at overlapping images. For example, they studied how the respondents thought about a past image (a horse) when looking at the backpack and the field. The researchers found the subjects who reactivated related memories while looking at overlapping image pairs were able to make associations between individual items (i.e. the horse and the field) despite the fact that they had never studied those images together.

    To illustrate the ways in which this cognitive process works, Preston describes an everyday scenario.

    Imagine you see a new neighbor walking a Great Dane down the street. At a different time and place, you may see a woman walking the same dog in the park. When experiencing the woman walking her dog, the brain conjures images of the recent memory of the neighbor and his Great Dane, causing an association between the dog walkers to be formed in memory. The derived relationship between the dog walkers would then allow you to infer the woman is also a new neighbor even though you have never seen her in your neighborhood.

    “This is just a simple example of how our brains store information that goes beyond the exact events we experience,” Preston says. “By combining past events with new information, we’re able to derive new knowledge and better anticipate what to expect in the future.”

    During the learning tasks, the researchers were able to pinpoint the brain regions that work in concert during the memory-binding process. They found the hippocampal-ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) circuit is essential for binding reactivated memories with current experience.

    For more information, contact: Jessica Sinn, College of Liberal Arts, 512-471-2404; Alison Preston, assistant professor, Departments of Psychology and Neurobiology, 512-475-7255, apreston@mail.clm.utexas.edu

    Summary:

    Memory enables flexible use of past experience to inform new behaviors. Although leading theories hypothesize that this fundamental flexibility results from the formation of integrated memory networks relating multiple experiences, the neural mechanisms that support memory integration are not well understood. Here, we demonstrate that retrieval-mediated learning, whereby prior event details are reinstated during encoding of related experiences, supports participants’ ability to infer relationships between distinct events that share content. Furthermore, we show that activation changes in a functionally coupled hippocampal and ventral medial prefrontal cortical circuit track the formation of integrated memories and successful inferential memory performance. These findings characterize the respective roles of these regions in retrieval-mediated learning processes that support relational memory network formation and inferential memory in the human brain. More broadly, these data reveal fundamental mechanisms through which memory representations are constructed into prospectively useful formats.

     

    Conclusions:

    Several leading theories hypothesize that the fundamental flexibility of episodic memory results from our ability to form networks of related memories that link discrete events (Buckner, 2010; Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993; O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978; Tolman, 1948). Despite the theoretical importance of this ques- tion, much empirical memory research has focused solely on en- coding processes that mediate memories for individual events. Here, we demonstrate that memories for distinct experiences are integrated through a retrieval-mediated encoding mecha- nism, with prior related memories being reactivated and bound to the current experience during encoding. Our data also highlight the importance of hippocampal interactions with VMPFC during the formation of such integrated memory networks, thus broadening our understanding of how these structures work in concert to support the flexibility of episodic memory. Together, these findings afford a deeper understanding of how remembering the past influences what we experience and learn in the present. More broadly, the results emphasize the adaptive nature of memory, whereby memory representations are constructed to anticipate, and successfully negotiate, future judgments.

                            author

    Stephanie West Allen

    Stephanie West Allen, JD, practiced law in California for several years, held offices in local bar associations, and wrote chapters for California Continuing Education of the Bar. While in CA, Stephanie completed several five-day mediation training programs with the Center for Mediation in Law, as well as a two-year intensive… MORE >

  • Featured Mediators

    ad
    View all

    Read these next

    Category

    Experts at Mediation: A Two-Edged Sword

    From the Blog of Phyllis G. Pollack.Sometimes, a party may have her expert participate in a mediation by being physically present and/or preparing a report for use at the mediation....

    By Phyllis Pollack
    Category

    Do Mediators Make Unsubstantiated Claims?

    Neuroscience and Conflict Resolution Blog by Stephanie West AllenIt is often a mystery to me why mediators will make statements about the practice of conflict resolution that are unproven or...

    By Stephanie West Allen
    Category

    Peace In; Peace Out

    Fairly Legal Blog by Clare FowlerI just watched the Season Premier of Fairly Legal and was caught off-guard by the demeanor of Kate, the mediator and main character. She was...

    By Clare Fowler

    Find a Mediator